a story lives forever
Register
Sign in
Form submission failed!

Stay signed in

Recover your password?
Register
Form submission failed!

Web of Stories Ltd would like to keep you informed about our products and services.

Please tick here if you would like us to keep you informed about our products and services.

I have read and accepted the Terms & Conditions.

Please note: Your email and any private information provided at registration will not be passed on to other individuals or organisations without your specific approval.

Video URL

You must be registered to use this feature. Sign in or register.

NEXT STORY

Open and closed science

RELATED STORIES

Somatic recombination theory
Gerald Edelman Scientist
Comments (0) Please sign in or register to add comments

After showing that we could recombine the chains, our task of course was to show how the individual amino acid sequence of each individual antibody differed from the next one, and that, we proposed, was the result of somatic recombination – an event that geneticists have obviously understood for many, many years of work outside of the soma in... in evolutionary time and in inheritance. So this recombination was proposed in the way of a theory that, inside each cell, recombination events caused the variable regions to vary. And, as I said before, this was three quarters right, but there was another addition which we hadn't explicitly mentioned – namely that there's also somatic mutation. So that meant there were special enzymes treating the DNA for antibodies.

It was an extraordinary piece of theoretical challenge and it was very pleasing because this was one case where theory mattered. Not only Macfarlane Burnet's theory but a whole bunch of people arguing about how you got the origin of diversity. The pun was that you needed a generator of diversity, G-O-D, and so everybody was looking for G-O-D. In fact, I had the privilege, in the middle of this somatic recombination theory of discussing it with my friend Jacques Monod, the great molecular biologist in... in France, at the Institut Pasteur. He and Jacob had invited me at this stage to give a lecture there and Monod and I particularly became close friends and we used to argue like crazy about the details of this... and I won't go into that now, but I do think it's worth a side bar to show how personalities differ and how people differ in respect to intellectual accomplishments.

We used to talk about different minds and one of the minds I brought up was Sigmund Freud, and he said, 'Gerry, Sigmund Freud is not only silly but he's a charlatan.' And I said, 'Jacques, what are you talking about? He wasn't a scientist even though he thought he was, but he was a great intellectual pioneer; he did more for understanding the notion of unconscious... the unconscious than anybody, even though other people had the idea before him.' And he said, 'Look, this is ridiculous.' And we had quite a heated exchange. At which point in exasperation I said, 'Jacques, all right, let's put it this way.' He said, 'Look, I have no unconscious; I'm responsible for everything I do.' By the way, he was of Huguenot extraction; I don't know if that had something to do with it. He said, 'I am responsible; there is no unconscious. This is Jacques.' And I finally said, 'Jacques, let's put it this way, everything Freud said applies to me and nothing to you.' And he said, 'Exactly, my dear fellow.' So, coming back, we used to argue about this origin of diversity and it remained quite a dilemma as to how to show it, until Susumu Tonegawa used molecular biological techniques to show how you had recombination of so-called V, D and J portions as well as somatic mutation occurring in the antibody genes.

US biologist Gerald Edelman (1929-2014) successfully constructed a precise model of an antibody, a protein used by the body to neutralise harmful bacteria or viruses and it was this work that won him the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1972 jointly with Rodney R Porter. He then turned his attention to neuroscience, focusing on neural Darwinism, an influential theory of brain function.

Listeners: Ralph J. Greenspan

Dr. Greenspan has worked on the genetic and neurobiological basis of behavior in fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) almost since the inception of the field, studying with one of its founders, Jeffery Hall, at Brandeis University in Massachusetts, where he received his Ph.D. in biology in 1979. He subsequently taught and conducted research at Princeton University and New York University where he ran the W.M. Keck Laboratory of Molecular Neurobiology, relocating to San Diego in 1997 to become a Senior Fellow in Experimental Neurobiology at The Neurosciences Institute. Dr. Greenspan’s research accomplishments include studies of physiological and behavioral consequences of mutations in a neurotransmitter system affecting one of the brain's principal chemical signals, studies making highly localized genetic alterations in the nervous system to alter behavior, molecular identification of genes causing naturally occurring variation in behavior, and the demonstration that the fly has sleep-like and attention-like behavior similar to that of mammals. Dr. Greenspan has been awarded fellowships from the Helen Hay Whitney Foundation, the Searle Scholars Program, the McKnight Foundation, the Sloan Foundation and the Klingenstein Foundation. In addition to authoring research papers in journals such as "Science", "Nature", "Cell", "Neuron", and "Current Biology", he is also author of an article on the subject of genes and behavior for "Scientific American" and several books, including "Genetic Neurobiology" with Jeffrey Hall and William Harris, "Flexibility and Constraint in Behavioral Systems" with C.P. Kyriacou, and "Fly Pushing: The Theory and Practice of Drosophila Genetics", which has become a standard work in all fruit fly laboratories.

Tags: Jacques Monod, Sigmund Freud, Susumu Tonegawa

Duration: 3 minutes, 5 seconds

Date story recorded: July 2005

Date story went live: 24 January 2008